By BOB SULLIVAN and HUGH THOMPSON
Published: May 3, 2013
TECHNOLOGY has given us many gifts, among them dozens of new ways to
grab our attention. It’s hard to talk to a friend without your phone
buzzing at least once. Odds are high you will check your Twitter feed or
Facebook wall while reading this article. Just try to type a memo at work without having an e-mail pop up that ruins your train of thought.
But what constitutes distraction? Does the mere possibility that a phone
call or e-mail will soon arrive drain your brain power? And does
distraction matter — do interruptions make us dumber? Quite a bit,
according to new research by Carnegie Mellon University’s Human-Computer Interaction Lab.
There’s a lot of debate among brain researchers about the impact of
gadgets on our brains. Most discussion has focused on the deleterious
effect of multitasking. Early results show what most of us know
implicitly: if you do two things at once, both efforts suffer.
In fact, multitasking is a misnomer. In most situations, the person
juggling e-mail, text messaging, Facebook and a meeting is really doing
something called “rapid toggling between tasks,” and is engaged in
constant context switching.
As economics students know, switching involves costs. But how much? When
a consumer switches banks, or a company switches suppliers, it’s
relatively easy to count the added expense of the hassle of change. When
your brain is switching tasks, the cost is harder to quantify.
There have been a few efforts to do so: Gloria Mark of the University of California, Irvine, found that a typical office worker gets only 11 minutes between each interruption, while it takes an average of 25 minutes to return
to the original task after an interruption. But there has been scant
research on the quality of work done during these periods of rapid
toggling.
We decided to investigate further, and asked Alessandro Acquisti, a professor of information technology,
and the psychologist Eyal Peer at Carnegie Mellon to design an
experiment to measure the brain power lost when someone is interrupted.
To simulate the pull of an expected cellphone call or e-mail, we had
subjects sit in a lab and perform a standard cognitive skill test. In
the experiment, 136 subjects were asked to read a short passage and
answer questions about it. There were three groups of subjects; one
merely completed the test. The other two were told they “might be
contacted for further instructions” at any moment via instant message.
During an initial test, the second and third groups were interrupted
twice. Then a second test was administered, but this time, only the
second group was interrupted. The third group awaited an interruption
that never came. Let’s call the three groups Control, Interrupted and On
High Alert.
We expected the Interrupted group to make some mistakes, but the results
were truly dismal, especially for those who think of themselves as
multitaskers: during this first test, both interrupted groups answered correctly 20 percent less often than members of the control group.
In other words, the distraction of an interruption, combined with the
brain drain of preparing for that interruption, made our test takers 20
percent dumber. That’s enough to turn a B-minus student (80 percent)
into a failure (62 percent).
But in Part 2 of the experiment, the results were not as bleak. This
time, part of the group was told they would be interrupted again, but
they were actually left alone to focus on the questions.
Again, the Interrupted group underperformed the control group, but this
time they closed the gap significantly, to a respectable 14 percent. Dr.
Peer said this suggested that people who experience an interruption,
and expect another, can learn to improve how they deal with it.
But among the On High Alert group, there was a twist. Those who were
warned of an interruption that never came improved by a whopping 43
percent, and even outperformed the control test takers who were left
alone. This unexpected, counterintuitive finding requires further
research, but Dr. Peer thinks there’s a simple explanation: participants
learned from their experience, and their brains adapted.
Somehow, it seems, they marshaled extra brain power to steel themselves
against interruption, or perhaps the potential for interruptions served
as a kind of deadline that helped them focus even better.
Clifford Nass, a Stanford sociologist who conducted some of the first
tests on multitasking, has said that those who can’t resist the lure of
doing two things at once are “suckers for irrelevancy.” There is some
evidence that we’re not just suckers for that new text message, or
addicted to it; it’s actually robbing us of brain power, too. Tweet
about this at your own risk.
What the Carnegie Mellon study shows, however, is that it is possible to
train yourself for distractions, even if you don’t know when they’ll
hit.